When it comes to karma, most people believe the universe is their own personal cheerleader — but an exacting judge for everyone else, according to a new study published May 1 by the American Psychological Association in the journal Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. Researchers asked more than 2,000 people from various religious and nonreligious backgrounds to write about their experiences with karma — the belief that good deeds are rewarded, and bad actions are punished — and a common through-line of their responses emerged. When the participants wrote about themselves, 59% told tales of being rewarded for their good deeds. In their stories about others, 92% chronicled how misfortune befell cheating partners, indebted friends, bullies and lousy coworkers. “Thinking about karma allows people to take personal credit and feel pride in good things that happen to them even when it isn’t clear exactly what they did to create the good outcome,” the study’s senior author, Cindel White, said in a statement. “But it also allows people to see other people’s suffering as justified retribution.” The findings offer a window into the way our minds form judgments about ourselves and those around us, said White, an assistant professor at York University in Toronto who studies how psychological motives interact with spirituality. “I firmly believe in providing charitable donations to those in need, no matter how small,” one anonymous response from the study reads. “Doing so has benefited me greatly and occasionally has led to individuals doing me favors without asking.” Another reads, “I knew a guy who was always rude and cruel to everyone; he never did anything nice his entire life. He was diagnosed with cancer, and I just couldn’t help but think that was karma.” What’s on your mind? The study identifies the tendency to see yourself as deserving of good fortune, even in the absence of direct causes, as a form of “attribution bias,” — a concept which has been studied by psychologists for decades, said Patrick Heck, a research psychologist at the Consumer Financial Protection Bureau, commenting in his personal capacity. “Attribution theory and attribution biases are the general idea that people attribute certain things that happen to them or other people in line with ways that make them feel good about themselves,” said Heck, who was not involved in the study. In some ways, attribution bias serves the purpose of promoting self-esteem, which helps people overcome challenges in life, White said. But other times, it could lead people astray when they fail to recognize the contributions of others or the external factors of success. On the other side of the coin, the belief that other people’s suffering is a form of punishment comes from a need to believe that the world is just. This belief can help people make sense of the complexity of life, Heck said. “(The world) is full of statistical noise. Random things happen to people all the time. And we know from lots of research in psychology that people struggle to reconcile randomness in their lives,” Heck said. “It’s very compelling to want to have a story or an explanation for why good things happen to some people and why bad things happen to other people. … I think karma is a really nicely packaged way that maybe has found its way into religion and other belief systems.” The cultural differences The concept of karma originates from Asian religious traditions such as Hinduism and Buddhism, and many variations exist in how it’s practiced between different groups, White said. For this reason, the study sampled participants from the United States, Singapore and India to get a broad range of cultural and religious backgrounds. Notably, results skewed slightly differently between Western and non-Western respondents, with Indian and Singaporean participants showing less self-enhancing bias — a finding that is consistent with previous research. “We found very similar patterns across multiple cultural contexts, including Western samples, where we know people often think about themselves in exaggeratedly positive ways, and samples from Asian countries where people are more likely to be self-critical,” White said. “But across all countries, participants were much more likely to say that other people face karmic punishments while they receive karmic rewards.” Broader implications of karma research White said her future research will explore how these beliefs around karma affect decision-making. “There’s sometimes differences between your religious beliefs, what people actually think about in everyday life, and then how they act,” White said. “There could be other situations where they are prompted by the experience itself, and they’re going to think about karma in a very different way.” Still, belief-driven biases can have broad, real-world implications, especially when it comes to policymaking and our justice system, Heck said. Often, social prejudices such as racism and classism function on the premise that an “inferior” group earned its status by behaving in a way that is socially unacceptable, said Yudit Jung, an adjunct associate professor of psychology at Emory University in Atlanta who was not involved in the research. Such views can then inform people’s treatment of those groups and their willingness to offer help. Ultimately, Jung, who is also a practicing psychoanalyst, said she believes the studied biases are common to most people. They can be a form of defense, originating from early childhood experiences and the need to feel secure. “I work with patients a lot on (developing) compassion and a sense of shared humanity, recognizing that we are all a mixture of good and bad,” Jung said. “It’s not about influencing religion. It’s about a patient’s basic ethics.”
Do bad people have it coming? Study finds most karma believers think so
TruthLens AI Suggested Headline:
"Study Reveals Insights on Beliefs About Karma and Their Impact on Perceptions of Justice"
TruthLens AI Summary
A recent study published by the American Psychological Association has revealed insights into how beliefs about karma influence people's perceptions of justice and personal merit. Conducted with over 2,000 participants from diverse religious and nonreligious backgrounds, the study found that while individuals often believe they are rewarded for their good deeds, they are more likely to perceive misfortune befalling others as just deserts for their wrongdoings. Specifically, 59% of respondents reported personal experiences of receiving benefits due to their positive actions, while an overwhelming 92% recounted stories of others facing negative outcomes, such as cheating partners or unkind colleagues. Cindel White, the study's senior author, noted that such beliefs allow individuals to take pride in their successes while rationalizing the suffering of others as deserved retribution, reflecting a psychological tendency known as attribution bias. This bias enables individuals to enhance their self-esteem, although it can sometimes obscure the contributions of external factors or the role of others in their success.
The research highlights cultural variations in the understanding of karma, drawing from participants in the United States, Singapore, and India. The findings indicate that while Western respondents tend to exhibit a stronger self-enhancing bias, individuals from non-Western cultures, particularly Indian and Singaporean participants, displayed a more tempered view of their own merits. This suggests that cultural context plays a significant role in shaping beliefs about karma and justice. Moreover, the study raises concerns about the broader implications of these biases, particularly in areas such as policymaking and social perceptions. Experts like Yudit Jung emphasize that these beliefs can reinforce societal prejudices, as individuals may attribute the plight of marginalized groups to their perceived moral failings. Ultimately, the study underscores the complexity of human judgment and the need for compassion and understanding in a world where randomness and injustice often coexist.
TruthLens AI Analysis
The recent study published in the journal Psychology of Religion and Spirituality sheds light on the widespread belief in karma, revealing fascinating insights into how individuals perceive their own actions in relation to others. This research offers a unique perspective on human psychology and societal values, particularly in how people attribute outcomes to moral behavior.
Perception of Self vs. Others
The study indicates a significant disparity in how people view their own fortunes compared to the misfortunes of others. While 59% of participants felt they were rewarded for their good deeds, an overwhelming 92% believed that others experienced deserved misfortune due to their bad actions. This reflects a common human tendency to engage in attribution bias, where individuals internalize positive outcomes while externalizing negative ones onto others. Such a mindset can reinforce a sense of moral superiority and justify a lack of empathy towards those who suffer.
Psychological Implications
The findings highlight the psychological mechanisms behind the belief in karma. Participants often found personal pride in their good outcomes, which may not always be directly linked to their actions. This suggests a coping strategy where individuals feel justified in their successes while rationalizing the failures of others. The study's senior author, Cindel White, emphasizes that this cognitive bias allows people to navigate their moral landscapes with a sense of fairness, albeit a skewed one.
Societal Reflections
The discussion surrounding karma touches on larger societal themes such as justice and morality. The belief in karmic retribution can create a divide in how communities perceive social issues. This study may inadvertently reinforce collective notions of justice, where the misfortunes of others are viewed as deserved, thus complicating discussions around compassion and support for those in need.
Potential for Manipulation
While the article presents research findings, there is a potential for manipulation in how these findings are interpreted and disseminated. The framing of karma as a universal truth could lead to a deterministic view of human behavior, which may overshadow the complexities of individual circumstances. By focusing on anecdotal evidence and attributing suffering to moral failings, the narrative could foster a lack of accountability for systemic issues that contribute to individuals' hardships.
Trustworthiness of the Research
The credibility of the study hinges on its methodology and the diversity of its participant pool. Although the research involves over 2,000 respondents, the qualitative nature of the responses may introduce biases based on individual experiences and cultural backgrounds. Thus, while the study provides valuable insights into the psychology of karma, it must be approached with a critical lens regarding its implications and applicability to broader societal contexts.
In summary, the study on karma reveals significant insights into human psychology and societal values, but it also raises questions about moral judgment and the potential for bias in perceiving justice. The implications of this research could affect societal attitudes towards compassion and accountability, necessitating further exploration into how these beliefs shape interpersonal dynamics.