‘Spiral of silence’: climate action is very popular, so why don’t people realise it?

TruthLens AI Suggested Headline:

"Study Reveals Widespread Support for Climate Action Despite Public Misperceptions"

View Raw Article Source (External Link)
Raw Article Publish Date:
AI Analysis Average Score: 7.5
These scores (0-10 scale) are generated by Truthlens AI's analysis, assessing the article's objectivity, accuracy, and transparency. Higher scores indicate better alignment with journalistic standards. Hover over chart points for metric details.

TruthLens AI Summary

A recent academic experiment highlighted the significant gap between public perception and actual support for climate action. Participants were asked how much of a $450 pot they would donate to a charity focused on reducing carbon emissions through renewable energy investments. The study revealed that while the average person donated about half, those informed that 79% of the public believed in the importance of climate action increased their donations significantly. This indicates that many individuals underestimate the level of support for climate initiatives, which may discourage them from contributing. The research conducted in the U.S. aligns with a broader global trend, suggesting that the misconception of climate action being unpopular could hinder collective efforts towards climate progress. Experts argue that correcting these misperceptions could catalyze a social tipping point, mobilizing the latent support for climate action into a robust movement.

The researchers conducted a comprehensive survey involving 130,000 people across 125 countries, revealing that an overwhelming 89% wanted their governments to take more action against climate change. Notably, while most participants believed that only 43% of their peers would be willing to contribute financially to climate efforts, the reality showed a much higher willingness. For instance, in China, 97% of respondents felt their government should do more, with a similar sentiment echoing in oil-rich nations like Saudi Arabia and the UAE. The findings underscore the existence of a 'silent majority' that actively supports climate initiatives but remains quiet due to misperceptions about public opinion. This disconnect is further exacerbated by misinformation campaigns that amplify the voices of climate change skeptics, making it crucial to communicate true social norms regarding climate action. By highlighting the widespread desire for change, experts believe that a concerted effort to correct public misconceptions can empower individuals and drive substantial policy changes at various levels of governance.

TruthLens AI Analysis

The article explores the concept of the "spiral of silence" in relation to climate action. It discusses a recent academic experiment that reveals a significant gap between public perception and actual support for climate initiatives. This discrepancy suggests that many individuals underestimate the popularity of climate action, which may affect their willingness to contribute to related causes. The findings highlight the potential of targeted communication campaigns to shift public perception and mobilize a latent climate movement.

Purpose of the Article

This piece aims to inform readers about the research findings and emphasize the importance of addressing misperceptions regarding public support for climate action. By shedding light on the "spiral of silence," the article advocates for a communication strategy that could encourage greater participation in climate initiatives, thereby potentially accelerating progress toward environmental goals.

Public Perception and Misconceptions

The article aims to create awareness about the common misconception that climate action lacks popular support. By presenting research that indicates a higher level of public concern for climate issues than previously believed, it encourages readers to reconsider their own views and actions regarding climate change. This is intended to foster a sense of community and shared responsibility among individuals who may feel isolated in their concerns.

Potential Omissions

While the article focuses on the positive implications of increased awareness, it may downplay the complexities of climate action, such as the political and economic barriers that exist. By concentrating on the psychological aspect of public perception, there may be an unintentional oversimplification of the challenges faced in addressing climate change.

Manipulative Elements

The article leans towards a manipulative narrative by presenting the research findings in a way that emphasizes the urgency and potential for change without adequately addressing the obstacles that still remain. The language used is optimistic and encourages a sense of collective action, which could be seen as a means to galvanize public support, but it might also gloss over the intricacies of climate policy and individual responsibility.

Overall Reliability

The article appears to be based on credible academic research, which lends it a level of reliability. However, the framing of the information and the emphasis on psychological factors may influence readers' understanding of the broader context of climate action. While the core findings are likely valid, the interpretation and implications presented could lead to an overly simplified view of a complex issue.

Impact on Society and Economy

This article could inspire increased public engagement in climate initiatives, potentially leading to a rise in donations to environmental causes and greater support for sustainable policies. By reshaping public perception, it may also influence political agendas and economic investments in renewable energy.

Target Audience

The article is likely to resonate with environmentally conscious communities, activists, and individuals interested in climate science. It appeals to those who may feel discouraged by previous perceptions of public support, providing them with a sense of empowerment to act.

Market Implications

The framing of climate action as broadly supported could impact market dynamics, particularly in sectors related to renewable energy and sustainability. Companies involved in green technologies might see a boost in investor confidence and public support, potentially affecting stock performance in these areas.

Geopolitical Context

In the current global landscape, discussions around climate change are increasingly relevant, as nations grapple with environmental commitments and public expectations. The article contributes to this discourse, underscoring the importance of recognizing collective support for climate action in shaping future policies and international agreements.

Artificial Intelligence Influence

There is no clear indication that AI was used in the writing of this article. However, if AI models were involved, they may have influenced the narrative style, emphasizing certain aspects of the research to align with broader communication strategies. This could include selecting language that resonates with readers and encourages action.

Conclusion

In conclusion, while the article presents valuable insights based on research findings, it is essential to approach its claims with a critical mindset. The optimism surrounding public support for climate action is compelling, but it is crucial to consider the complexities and barriers that exist in the broader context of environmental policy and action.

Unanalyzed Article Content

How much of a $450 (£339) pot would you give to a charity that cuts carbon emissions by investing in renewable energy, and how much would you keep for yourself? That was the question posed in a recent academic experiment. The answers mattered: real money was handed out as a result to some randomly chosen participants.

The average person gave away about half the money and kept the rest. But what if you had been told beforehand that the vast majority of other people think climate action is really important? Might you have given more to the charity?

That is what a second experiment tested. Before dividing the cash, these participants were told that 79% of people thought citizens should try to fight the climate crisis. This mattered, because in earlier questions people had significantly underestimated the proportion at just 61%. Being informed about the true level of support boosted the donations by $16 for each person.

The experiment was in the US, but the illusion that climate action is not popular is global. So imagine dispelling that myth: such a shift, experts say, could be a gamechanger, pushing the world over a social tipping point into unstoppable climate progress.

Such a communication campaign, low-cost and scalable, could be among the most powerful tools available to fight the climate crisis, they say. Decades of psychological research indicates that correcting such misunderstandings can change people’s views across a swathe of issues, from participating in protests to voting for Donald Trump.

“We’re sitting on an enormous potential climate movement,” said Prof Anthony Leiserowitz, at Yale University in the US. “It’s latent. It hasn’t been activated or catalysed. But when you break through these perception gaps, you help people understand that they’re not alone and there is in fact a global movement.”

The research started with a simple goal, says Prof Teodora Boneva, at the University of Bonn, Germany, who with colleagues undertook the experiments: “We wanted to make a difference to the world. So we asked ourselves, as social scientists and economists: what kind of research can we do?”

Their biggest result was a huge, globe-spanning survey that revealed the remarkable fact that people across the world are united in wanting action to fight the climate crisis but remain a silent majority, because theywrongly think only a minority share their views.

“Before, just like everyone else, we would have underestimated the support,” she says. “So when we saw the numbers coming in, we thought: ‘Wow!’ We had no idea we would find such consistent patterns in so many countries.”

The team found 89% of people across the world wanted their national governments to do more to fight global heating. More than two-thirds said they were willing to give 1% of their income to fight the climate crisis. Crucially, however, they thought only a minority of other people – 43% – would be willing to do the same.

The survey involved 130,000 people in 125 countries, which account for 96% of the world’s carbon emissions, and waspublished in the journal Nature Climate Change. People in China, the world’s biggest polluter, were among the most concerned, with 97% saying its government should do more to fight the climate crisis and four out of five willing to give 1% of their income. The world’s second biggest polluter, the US, was near the bottom, but still had three-quarters of its citizens saying its government should do more and almost half willing to contribute.

Even in the petrostates of Saudi Arabia and the United Arab Emirates, the vast majority – about 80% – were willing to give 1% of their income to climate action. How many of these wanted more government action is unknown: these countries did not allow the question to be asked.

Those feeling the heat most directly had the strongest pro-climate views. Those in rich countries were significantly less willing to contribute 1%. And the countries where people most strongly wanted to fight the climate crisis had implemented significantly more climate policies, the researchers found. The more strongly people believed their fellow citizens would contribute their money to climate action, the more likely they were to give themselves, as Boneva’s later experiments with the $450 pots showed.

“These positive interactions suggest that a change in one factor can unlock potent, self-reinforcing feedback cycles, triggering social-tipping dynamics,” the team concluded. “Our results suggest a concerted effort to correct these misperceptions could be a powerful intervention, yielding large, positive effects.”

A single survey, such as Boneva’s, is fascinating – but perhaps you are thinking it could be a freak result. It is not. Many large studies have shown that the public desire for climate action is deep and global, and that the misperceptions that fuel a “spiral of silence” on climate are found wherever researchers look.

A UN poll in 2024 – dubbed thePeople’s Climate Vote– questioned 75,000 people in countries representing 90% of the global population. It found 80% wanted their countries to strengthen their climate commitments. Another recentsurvey of 40,000 peoplein 20 of the world’s most polluting countries found that 86% of people thought the same.

How the question is phrased can change, but people’s hunger for climate action does not. Asurvey of 140,000 people(pdf) in 187 countries and territories by the Yale Program on Climate Change Communication asked how high a priority climate change should be for the government of their country: 89% of people said very high, high or medium, with 67% saying very high or high.

The perception gaps are real too. A US study from 2022 found people thought onlyabout 40% of their fellow citizens supported climate policies: the real proportion was about 75%. An earlier study found thesame in China.

Other commonly held beliefs have been revealed as false, including the idea that people in rich nations are unwilling to give money to poorer nations to help fight the climate crisis. Astudy testing support for the redistribution of moneyfrom a global emissions trading scheme from the rich to the poor found the scheme was backed by 76% of Europeans and 54% of those in the US.

“There is a silent majority in favour of [action on] climate change,” said Dr Adrien Fabre, at the International Centre for Research on Environment and Development in France, who led the study. “Making people aware of this would help a lot and bring hope. I think a lot of people are self-censoring and not fighting or voting [for climate action] because they think that their ideas are not in the zeitgeist.”

“There is also majority support for global redistribution policies, even among people in high-income countries who understand that they would have to pay for it,” said Fabre. “This is because people attach value to the human rights and wellbeing of fellow human beings and to a stable climate.”

But is this huge support for climate action in surveys actually sincere, or is it just people saying what they think the researchers want to hear?

Fabre, Boneva and other academics routinely test the reliability of information from surveys using numerous tools. These include comparing people’s answers with their real-world behaviour, for example, finding correlations between people’s stated willingness to pay money for climate action and their actual donations to climate charities.

Other tests link their answers to real-world consequences, such as telling them their answers would be sent to the government, or by hiding the climate questions among others so the respondents cannot guess the “right” answer.

In any case, Fabre said: “Polls are rarely wrong by more than 5%.” That can make a big difference in tight elections, but not when talking about large majorities such as the 89% wanting more climate action from their governments.

Do you think politicians suffer under the same illusions about the popularity of climate action as the public? You might think their political antennas are finely tuned to public opinion, but they are not – sometimes wildly underestimating public views. And that matters, said Dr Niall McLoughlin, at Climate Barometer, which analyses climate opinions in the UK.

The group found in 2024 that72% of the UK public supported onshore windbeing built in their areas, but only 19% of MPs thought a majority of their constituents did so. “That was a barrier to the approval and rollout of developments,” the analysts concluded. Similar results have beenfound in the US Congresson carbon regulations.

Climate action is backed even by those who vote for political parties that are explicitly opposed to it, according to the UK’s More in Common thinktank. The rightwing populist Reform UK party recently labelled climate concern “hysteria” but 62% of thepeople who voted for the party in 2024said it was important that the government cares about tackling climate change.

The apparent contradiction is explained by the fact that very few (4%) Reform voters said the party got their vote because of its environmental policies. Instead, 72% said it was because of its hardline anti-immigration policies.

Boneva said: “Informing the policy debate on climate issues can be very helpful. If we make politicians more aware of what the people in their country want, they might actually act on the people’s preferences.”

Most people are social creatures, highly influenced by what others do and say. That is why correcting mistaken beliefs about the views of your fellow citizens can make an impact on what you think and do. There is alot of researchshowing this can shift people’s views on a range of social justice issues.

First, people are drawn towards “social norms”, the perceived standard of acceptable behaviour. “There is a really strong motive among people to try to adhere to what they think is socially desirable,” said Boneva. “This has been shown in many, many different contexts.”

People are also “conditional cooperators”: they are more likely to contribute to the public good if they think others are doing the same. “This motive has also been studied very extensively,” Boneva said. “If everyone else in a shared home is cleaning the dishes, you will do it too. If everyone else is just leaving their stuff, you won’t bother either.”

Studieshave shown thattelling peoplehow popular environmental action is with others can increase their own actions, for example insaving energyandgreen living. Correctingmisperceptions among US citizensabout the climate views of people in China increased support for a climate treaty.

However, studies specifically on green issues are relatively rare to date and have shown mixed results, said Dr Sandra Geiger, at Princeton University in the US. Her soon-to-be published work in 11 countries did show asmall increase in people’s willingnessto talk about the climate crisis when they were told a majority agreed with them.

That could be important, she said: “Correcting these misperceptions can disrupt thisspiral of silence. If you then believe that a majority agrees with you, you’re more likely to talk about the issue and this, in turn, corrects misperceptions in other people.”

But correcting the misperception did not lead to people putting more priority on government climate action, or living greener lives themselves, Geiger said. Anotherrecent study in 63 countriesfound correcting misperceptions had no impact on people’s climate views, as did another on the acceptance ofcarbon taxes.

The impact of dispelling mistaken views might depend on how it is corrected, for example, using stories rather than data, or correcting repeatedly not just once. “We haven’t really found out yet,” said Geiger.Other barriers might also remain, such as entrenched ideologies.

But she highlighted one approach clearly shown to work: correcting people’s misperception about theproportion of climate scientiststhat agree that global heating is human-caused and a serious problem, which is extremely high.

Despite the varying results to date, Geiger still backs campaigns to correct misperceptions: “People deserve to know, and campaigns are cheap and scalable.”

Why do these collective illusions about the level of support for climate action exist at all? What is the root cause? The answer remains uncertain but is likely to be a complex mix of human psychology – people tend to think worse of others than themselves, for example – and fossil-fuelled disinformation.

“One reason is just fundamentally human, because we can’t easily read the hearts and minds of our fellow human beings,” said Leiserowitz. “It’s made doubly hard because climate change is an issue that is abstract and often cloaked in scientific information.”

“A third critical reason is the existence of a very large, sophisticated, well-funded and longstanding misinformation campaign being driven by the fossil fuel industry and its allies, sowing doubt and division to maintain their profits,” he said.

This campaign has acted as an “enormous megaphone” for the small but vocal minority that dismiss climate science – about 10% in the US – he said: “As a result, they tend to dominate the public square. Climate change has joined sex, religion and politicsas a taboo topicat the Thanksgiving Day table. So communicating social norms is one of the most powerful interventions you can make.”

What about today’s rapidly changing geopolitics, with rising nationalism, Trump entrenching climate denial in the US, and the threat of trade wars and recessions? This makes the data on people’s real climate views more important than ever, said Cassie Flynn, the global director of climate change at the UN Development Programme, which ran the People’s Climate Vote.

“People are actually very multilateral,” she said. “The result that always sticks with me is this: 86% of people thought that countries should put aside their differences on other issues and work together. People understand our fates are tied together when it comes to the climate crisis and they want world leaders to act on it.”

And everyone can help break the “spiral of silence”,according toProf Cynthia Frantz, at Oberlin College in the US: “[Change] simply requires that people be exposed, over and over, from sources they trust or identify with, to the fact that they are not alone in their concern and their willingness to take action.”

Frantz said: “The empowering truth is that every public statement counts, and the more diverse the voices, the more effective the message will be.”

This story is part ofThe 89 Percent Project, an initiative of the global journalism collaborationCovering Climate Now.

Back to Home
Source: The Guardian